Research Paper Volume 12, Issue 18 pp 18660—18675

β-lactolin increases cerebral blood flow in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy adults: a randomized controlled trial

Yasuhisa Ano1, , Keiko Kobayashi1, , Mamoru Hanyuda2, , Ryuta Kawashima3,4, ,

  • 1 Kirin Central Research Institute, Kirin Holdings Company, Ltd., Kanazawa-ku, Yokohama 236-0004, Japan
  • 2 Breast Health Clinic, Chuo-ku, Tokyo 103-0025, Japan
  • 3 Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer (IDAC), Tohoku University, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-0872, Japan
  • 4 NeU Corporation, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 101-0048, Japan

Received: June 9, 2020       Accepted: August 8, 2020       Published: September 29, 2020      

https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.103951
How to Cite

Copyright: © 2020 Ano et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Abstract

The number of elderly individuals with age-related cognitive decline or dementia is rapidly increasing. Dairy product consumption, including β-lactolin, is beneficial for their cognitive function. The underlying mechanism of β-lactolin’s effects on human brain activity is yet to be investigated. We examined the β-lactolin effects on human cerebral blood flow (CBF) using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) in a placebo-controlled randomized double-blind study, which reported according to the CONSORT guidelines. Fifty healthy participants (aged 45–60 years) were randomly allocated into the β-lactolin or the placebo group (n = 25 each) and supplemented for 6 weeks. During the 6th week, oxy-hemoglobin during the working memory tasks was measured using 34-channels (CHs) NIRS. The changes of oxy-hemoglobin, which represents the CBF, in CH 23 located at the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during the spatial working memory task showed higher statistical significance (false discovery rate (q) = 0.045) in the β-lactolin than in the placebo group. The oxy-Hb changes in CH23 have a co-relationship with the working memory task reaction time. This clinical trial showed an increase in the CBF in the left DLPFC area during the 6-week β-lactolin supplementation. This study contributes to elucidating the underlying mechanisms of β-lactolin on cognitive performance.

Introduction

The global number of people with age-related cognitive decline or dementia is rapidly increasing, due to the growing aging population. Previous epidemiological studies have shown that daily consumption of dairy products prevents age-related cognitive decline [14]. Some studies have demonstrated that dairy products prevent cognitive decline in rodents and humans [5, 6]. Indeed, camembert cheese, a dairy product fermented with fungi, prevents the development of Alzheimer’s disease in a transgenic mouse model [7]. Our group identified β-lactopeptide, tryptophan-tyrosine (WY)-related lactopeptides including WY and glycine-threonine-tryptophan-tyrosine (GTWY), which improve cognitive and mood function and prevent Alzheimer’s disease pathology [5, 8, 9]. Especially, β-lactolin of GTWY peptide derived from β-lactoglobulin is rich in fermented dairy products including camembert cheese, blue cheese, and whey enzymatic digestions [5, 10]. Orally administered β-lactolin is delivered to the brain and inhibits the activity of monoamine oxidase B (MAO-B) in mice, thus, increasing the dopamine levels in the cortex and the hippocampus [11, 12]. β-lactolin improves spatial working and episodic object recognition memory in pharmacologically induced amnesia in young and aged mice [5]. β-lactolin also improves memory impairment and suppresses Alzheimer’s pathologies including Aβ-deposition and inflammation in the brain in Alzheimer’s transgenic 5×FAD mice [13].

In addition to preclinical studies’ demonstrations, our previous clinical study showed that supplementation with β-lactolin for 6 weeks improves the score of the verbal fluency and Stroop tests, evaluating the executive function and the inhibition of attention in healthy middle-aged adults, respectively, compared with the placebo group in a randomized placebo-controlled trial [14]. We also demonstrated that the supplementation with β-lactolin improves selective and sustained attention (evaluated by a visual-cancellation test) and the working memory (evaluated by paired-associate learning tests in healthy older adults) compared with the placebo group in a randomized placebo-controlled trial [6]. These previous demonstrations showed that supplementing with β-lactolin improves attention, executive function, and memory retrieval especially associated with the function of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). The DLPFC is an area in the prefrontal cortex of the brain, which is essential for the executive function, such as working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, planning, and abstract reasoning. Thus, the activation of DLPFC is associated with these cognitive functions [1517]. The impairment of the DLPFC’s function is associated with cognitive decline in elderly patients and those with dementia; therefore, supplementation of β-lactolin might be expected to prevent cognitive decline and dementia. Conversely, the association of β-lactolin supplementation with DLPFC activation is yet to be evaluated.

Prefrontal cortex cerebral nerve activity is evaluated using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), and near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). NIRS is a non-invasive and easy-to-use evaluation; therefore, the number of clinical trials using NIRS to measure the DLPFC’s function has been increasing in healthy participants [18]. Compared with MRI and SPECT, NIRS measures the regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) on the cortical surface. The region of the DLPFC, activated by β-lactolin supplementation, is the superficial cortex, of which the rCBF is detectable by the NIRS. Recent studies using NIRS have assessed cortical activation and functional connectivity during a visuospatial working memory task and auditory working memory, measuring the activity of the DLPFC during memory encoding and retrieval [1921]. These demonstrations showed that the measurement using NIRS is available for the evaluation of the DLPFC activity.

In this study, we conducted a randomized placebo-controlled trial and measured the DLPFC’s activity during cognitive tasks to evaluate the effects of β-lactolin on the rCBF in the DLPFC. We measured the oxy-hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) levels during the working memory tasks by multi-channel (CH) 34CH NIRS at 0 and 6 weeks of the interventions to evaluate the DLPFC’s activation. To elucidate the underlying mechanism of β-lactolin on the previously demonstrated cognitive function, we measured the rCBF in healthy older adults, similar to the previously performed works. These demonstrations would contribute to elucidate the associations between the DLPFC’s activation and the rCBF with improved cognitive function by the supplementation of β-lactolin.

Results

Baseline characteristics of the study group

The flow chart of the participant selection process is presented in Figure 1. We recruited 71 Japanese-speaking healthy older adults aged between 45–60 years (male/female = 35/36). Following screening, 50 participants (male/female = 26/24) were included and 21 participants were excluded as they withdrew their participation (n = 2), met the exclusion criteria (n = 12), had suspended dementia (n = 2), were left-handed (n = 1), had suspected hay fever (n = 1) and irregular lifestyle (n = 2), and participated in other clinical trials (n = 1). The 50 participants were randomly allocated to the β-lactolin and placebo groups and were supplemented for 6 weeks. One participant in the β-lactolin group did not visit at week 6 of the intervention period and excluded. Finally, the data of 24 (male/female = 13/11) and 25 participants (male/female = 13/12) in the β-lactolin and the placebo group, respectively, were analyzed (Table 1).

CONSORT diagram. We included 50 of the 71 screened participants in this study; they were randomly allocated to the placebo (n = 25) and β-lactolin (n = 25) groups. One participant dropped out and, therefore, the data of 25 and 24 participants in the placebo and β-lactolin groups were analyzed, respectively.

Figure 1. CONSORT diagram. We included 50 of the 71 screened participants in this study; they were randomly allocated to the placebo (n = 25) and β-lactolin (n = 25) groups. One participant dropped out and, therefore, the data of 25 and 24 participants in the placebo and β-lactolin groups were analyzed, respectively.

Table 1. Participants’ characteristics at baseline.

CharacteristicsPlaceboβ-lactolinp
Age52.2 ± 4.352.2 ± 4.90.996
Male/Female13 / 1213 / 110.879
BMI22.7 ± 4.122.9 ± 3.70.894
MMSE29.3 ± 1.129.0 ± 1.10.457
Data are presented as means ± SD: the placebo (n = 25) and the β-lactolin (n = 24). p-values were calculated using unpaired t-tests, except for those for the male/female ratios, who were calculated using χ2 tests.
BMI, body mass index; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; SD, standard deviation.

rCBF measured using a 34CH NIRS (Primary outcome)

At intervention weeks 0 and 6, the rCBF during working memory tasks was measured by the 34CH NIRS. The changes in the mean z-score of oxy-Hb for each task at the region of interest (ROI) are shown in Tables 2 and 3 and in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2. The baseline oxy-Hb levels in CH11, CH13, CH14, CH20, CH22, and CH23 at week 0 of the intervention did not differ between the groups (analysis of variance [ANOVA]; q = 0.456, 0.701, 0.456, 0.701, 0.456, and 0.456 [verbal working memory] and q = 0.788, 0.987, 0.437, 0.915, 0.431, and 0.126 [spatial working memory] at a respective channel). Regarding the oxy-Hb at the CH23 in the spatial working memory tasks at week 6 of the intervention, the two-way ANOVA showed statistically significant effects of interaction between group and time (q = 0.045, Table 3 and Figure 2). The changes of oxy-Hb in other channels, except for CH14, were higher in the β-lactolin than the placebo group in the verbal and spatial working memory tasks (Tables 2 and 3). There was no statistically significant difference in the oxy-Hb levels at week 0 of the baseline. The topography during the spatial working memory at week 6 of the intervention is shown in Figure 3. CH 23 was located at the left DLPFC area, thus, indicating that supplementation with β-lactolin increases the rCBF in the DLPFC during spatial working memory tasks. Conversely, there was no statistically significant difference in the oxy-Hb levels in the serial 7 subtractions (serial 7s) and rapid visual information processing (RVIP) tasks (data not shown).

Changes of the oxy-Hb levels during spatial working memory in the left DLPFC. Oxy-Hb measurement at weeks 0 and 6 and changes between weeks 0 and 6 in CH23 during the spatial working memory task. Data are presented as means and ± standard errors for the placebo (n = 25) and the β-lactolin group (n = 24). Group differences were identified using ANOVA and FDR correlation. A value q

Figure 2. Changes of the oxy-Hb levels during spatial working memory in the left DLPFC. Oxy-Hb measurement at weeks 0 and 6 and changes between weeks 0 and 6 in CH23 during the spatial working memory task. Data are presented as means and ± standard errors for the placebo (n = 25) and the β-lactolin group (n = 24). Group differences were identified using ANOVA and FDR correlation. A value q < 0.05 considered statistically significant. ANOVA, analysis of variance; CH, channel; Hb, hemoglobin; FDR, false discovery rate; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Topographic images of oxy-Hb during spatial working memory. (A and B) The topographic maps during the spatial working memory task for oxy-Hb reveal the distribution of rCBF at week 6 of the intervention for both groups (placebo and β-lactolin, respectively). Hb, hemoglobin; rCBF, regional cerebral blood flow.

Figure 3. Topographic images of oxy-Hb during spatial working memory. (A and B) The topographic maps during the spatial working memory task for oxy-Hb reveal the distribution of rCBF at week 6 of the intervention for both groups (placebo and β-lactolin, respectively). Hb, hemoglobin; rCBF, regional cerebral blood flow.

Table 2. Oxy-Hb measurement in DLPFC during verbal working memory task.

GroupNWeek 0qWeek 6qΔANOVA q
CH11Placebo240.50 ± 0.650.4560.40 ± 0.660.991-0.11 ± 1.090.493
β-lactolin240.19 ± 0.900.40 ± 0.750.21 ± 1.13
CH13Placebo250.61 ± 0.890.7010.82 ± 0.790.8890.21 ± 1.180.493
β-lactolin230.49 ± 0.710.94 ± 1.17*0.46 ± 0.92
CH14Placebo23-0.14 ± 0.940.4560.04 ± 0.700.8890.18 ± 1.230.493
β-lactolin240.13 ± 0.78-0.02 ± 0.57-0.15 ± 0.81
CH20Placebo240.31 ± 1.370.7010.34 ± 1.220.8890.03 ± 2.070.72
β-lactolin240.15 ± 1.440.21 ± 1.160.06 ± 1.50
CH22Placebo250.47 ± 0.750.4560.63 ± 0.860.8890.16 ± 1.030.493
β-lactolin240.09 ± 0.860.49 ± 0.730.40 ± 1.34
CH23Placebo250.14 ± 0.650.4560.10 ± 0.610.889-0.05 ± 1.030.493
β-lactolin24-0.05 ± 0.670.25 ± 0.730.30 ± 1.09
The Z-score of Oxy-Hb measurement at weeks 0 and 6, and changes between weeks 0 and 6 in six channels (CH11, CH13, CH14, CH20, CH22, and CH23) during the verbal working memory task. Data are presented as means ± SD. The number of participants in each channel was represented. Group differences were identified using ANOVA and FDR correlation. Statistically significant differences between baseline and week 6 were identified using paired t-tests.
ANOVA, analysis of variance; CH, channel; Hb, hemoglobin; FDR, false discovery rate; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Table 3. Oxy-Hb measurement in the DLPFC during spatial working memory task.

GroupNWeek 0qWeek 6qΔANOVA q
CH11Placebo250.30 ± 0.610.7880.09 ± 0.930.337-0.20 ± 1.170.26
β-lactolin240.19 ± 0.570.48 ± 0.740.30 ± 0.85
CH13Placebo250.47 ± 0.940.9870.36 ± 0.980.337-0.10 ± 1.220.26
β-lactolin230.47 ± 0.810.86 ± 1.170.39 ± 0.99
CH14Placebo23-0.35 ± 0.870.437-0.23 ± 0.930.5430.12 ± 1.070.585
β-lactolin24-0.03 ± 0.86-0.04 ± 0.800.00 ± 0.99
CH20Placebo240.00 ± 1.570.915-0.16 ± 1.170.543-0.16 ± 2.070.610
β-lactolin230.16 ± 2.060.14 ± 1.25-0.02 ± 1.96
CH22Placebo250.46 ± 0.740.4310.54 ± 0.800.7750.07 ± 0.950.452
β-lactolin240.15 ± 0.740.47 ± 0.820.32 ± 0.99
CH23Placebo250.36 ± 0.810.1260.18 ± 0.610.424-0.17 ± 0.970.045
β-lactolin24-0.22 ± 0.900.37 ± 0.37**0.59 ± 0.94
Oxy-Hb measurement at weeks 0 and 6, and changes between week 0 and week 6 in 6 channels (CH11, CH13, CH14, CH20, CH22 and CH23) during the spatial working memory task. Data are presented as means and ± SD. The number of participants in each channel was represented. Group differences were identified using ANOVA and FDR correlation. Statistically significant differences between baseline and week 6 were identified using paired t-tests. A value of q < 0.05 is considered statistically significant.
ANOVA, analysis of variance; CH, channel; Hb, hemoglobin; FDR, false discovery rate; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Cognitive tasks

Changes in task performance in the verbal and spatial working memory tasks during the rCBF measurement were shown in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2, respectively. Changes in the shortest reaction time in verbal working memory tasks showed a statistically significant reduction at week 6 of intervention in the β-lactolin group, compared with those at baseline (paired t-test; p = 0.023). This reduction was not observed in the placebo group (Supplementary Table 1). Changes in the average reaction time in spatial working memory tasks tended to be reduced at week 6 of intervention in the β-lactolin group, compared with those at baseline (paired t-test; p = 0.056) (Supplementary Table 2). Conversely, there were no statistically significant differences between the groups. The cognitive task levels are supposedly adequate to induce an rCBF increase with undetectable cognitive function differences, as the correct percentages in the verbal and spatial working memory tasks were more than 89–95 % (data not shown).

Correlation between task performance and cerebral blood flow

The correlation between the change in the reaction time and the oxy-Hb levels at CH23 during the spatial working memory task is shown in the Supplementary Figure 1. We performed Spearman’s rank correlation for changes at 6 weeks to investigate the correlation between task performance and the left DLPFC’s activity during the spatial working memory task, which showed a statistically significant increase in the oxy-Hb levels. The reaction time and the oxy-Hb changes tended to be negatively correlated at CH23 (Spearman’s rank correlation; r = -0.27, p = 0.064) (Supplementary Figure 1) and showed a statistically significant negative correlation with those at CH 20 (Spearman’s rank correlation; r = -0.32, p = 0.030, data not shown). This result indicated that the left DLPFC’s oxy-Hb is positively correlated with task performance (reduction of average reaction time).

Discussion

This is the first study to demonstrate the effects of β-lactolin on the rCBF during cognitive tasks using a NIRS. Supplementation with β-lactolin increased the rCBF in the DLPFC area during working memory tasks, compared with the placebo group in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. These results suggested that supplementation with β-lactolin promotes DLPFC activity and improves the DLPFC-associated cognitive functions including memory retrieval, attention, and executive function.

The DLPFC is an area in the prefrontal cortex that plays an important role in the executive functions for the management of cognitive processes [22], such as working memory, cognitive flexibility [23], planning [24], inhibition, and abstract reasoning. Previous randomized controlled trials performed by our team demonstrated that supplementation with β-lactolin improved the executive function [14], selective and sustained attention [6], and associative working memory [6] in healthy older adults, as evaluated using the verbal fluency, Stroop, visual cancellation, and paired-associated tests, respectively. These functions are closely related to the function of the DLPFC. It is reported that damage to the DLPFC impairs working memory [25], attention [26], planning [27], motivation to perform self-improvement and social activities, and causes disinterest in their surroundings [28]. Dysfunction of the DLPFC is associated with schizophrenia [29], depression [30], stress [31], and Alzheimer’s disease [32]. Such dysfunction also reduced the rCBF in the DLPFC, especially its left side. It has been reported that rCBF activation was impaired in the left DLPFC and the number of task errors increased with aging, during the card sorting test in the elderly [33]. It has also been reported using SPECT that the rCBF in the left DLPFC is associated with full-scale intelligence quotient, perceptual reasoning, and processing speed in patients with moyamoya disease [34]. In addition, a report stated that the rCBF in the left DLPFC is required for the spatial rather than the verbal working memory in the elderly [35]. Taken together, it means that increased rCBF in the left DLPFC area produced by supplementation with β-lactolin is associated with improved cognitive function in older adults, as demonstrated in previous studies [6, 14]. Conversely, in this study, the rCBF in the DLPFC, during serial 7s and RVIP, was not changed by the supplementation of β-lactolin. Serial 7s and RVIP in the trial increased the rCBF in all area channels. In the z-score analysis, the rCBF at the DLPFC areas was relatively low and could not have been adequately evaluated. Serial 7s and RVIP tasks require attention associated with the function of the parietal and the frontal area. A report indicated that the RVIP task activates the rCBF in the fronto-parietal lobes in the experiment using positron emission tomography [36], potentially making it difficult to effectively evaluate the DLPFC’s activation by these tasks with the multi-channel NIRS. To conclude the mechanisms of neural activation by β-lactolin supplementation, further studies are needed to investigate the effects of β-lactolin on the DLPFC and in other fronto-parietal areas using adequate cognitive tasks.

Recent studies demonstrated that transcranial direct current stimulation to the DLPFC improves working memory, attention inhibition, and executive function in patients with Alzheimer’s disease [3739]. Repeated stimulation to the left DLPFC using transcranial magnetic stimulation also improves depressive and anxious symptoms in medication-naïve patients with depression [40]. Conversely, such stimulations are required during surgery; however, they have not been approved as a therapy. Other non-invasive approaches to activate the DLPFC have gained increased attention. A recent study showed that working memory-training improves functional connectivity and the rCBF during rest in a clinical trial [41]. Some nutrients improve the rCBF in the frontal cortex in a clinical trial. For instance, it is reported that supplementation with Ginkgo biloba in older adults promotes rCBF and cognitive performance [42]. Conversely, there are limited approaches to promote rCBF in the DLPFC. Supplementation with β-lactolin will be a promising approach to promote rCBF in the DLPFC and improve cognitive function in the elderly.

The functions of the DLPFC are activated by neurotransmitters including dopamine, serotonin, norepinephrine, and Gamma-aminobutyric acid. Deficient neurotransmitters impair working memory, planning, inhibition, and decision making, especially dopamine, which is distributed mostly in the frontal cortex and is associated with its functions [43]. Aging reduces the dopamine levels in the frontal cortex, leading to cognitive decline [44]. Treatments that increase the dopamine levels improve cognitive function. The dopamine D1 receptors are mostly involved in working memory tasks [45]. Their levels in the frontal and parietal cortices are reduced by age and are associated with impaired working memory performance [46]. Additionally, in patients with Parkinson’s disease, L-dopa ameliorates high-level cognitive deficits by increasing the rCBF in the DLPFC, thus, suggesting that the dopamine system is involved in cognitive function improvement in the DLPFC [47]. Our previous preclinical studies demonstrated that oral β-lactolin administration activated the dopaminergic neurons and increased the dopamine levels in the cortex and the hippocampus, resulting in improved spatial working memory and object recognition memory in a rodent model. Orally administered β-lactolin is delivered to the brain through the blood-brain barrier and inhibits MAO-B, increases the dopamine levels, and improves impaired memory [5, 48]. The blockade of the dopamine D1 receptor attenuates the effects of β-lactolin to improve cognitive function in pharmacologically induced amnesiac mice. Moreover, our preclinical study showed that the dopamine levels in the cortex were reduced in Alzheimer’s disease model 5×FAD mice. Treatment with β-lactolin or Trp-Tyr of its core sequence increased the cerebral dopamine levels, reduced the cerebral amyloid β levels, and improved the impaired object recognition memory [8, 13]. These reports underscored the benefits of β-lactolin for improving the dopamine levels and increasing rCBF in the DLPFC via the dopaminergic system, thereby attenuating cognitive decline and dementia.

There were several limitations in this study. The rCBF in the left DLPFC area during the spatial working memory was significantly higher in the β-lactolin than in the placebo group but the verbal working memory was higher though not statistically significant. This trial indicated that β-lactolin supplementation activates the left DLPFC function; however, the area increased by β-lactolin was limited to the left side of the DLPFC (CH23). To conclude the effects of β-lactolin on the rCBF of the DLPFCs during working memory tasks, further studies are required to evaluate the effects of β-lactolin on the DLPFC activities during working memory tasks, involving a larger study population using NIRS, MRI, or SPECT.

In conclusion, this study is the first to demonstrate that supplementation with β-lactolin increases the rCBF in the left DLPFC area during working memory tasks. As such, supplementation with β-lactolin may be an effective approach to improve rCBF and promote cognitive function associated with the DLPFC, leading to prevention from dementia.

Materials and Methods

Ethics and registration

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and Ethical Guidelines for Medical and Health Research Involving Human Participants. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. This trial was approved by the ethics committee of the Japanese Conference of Clinical Research (JCCR, Tokyo, Japan) and registered on the 11th of January 2019 in the database of UMIN prior to enrollment (Registration No. UMIN000035521; Registration title. A study to evaluate the impact of test food ingestion on rCBF).

Participants

We recruited 71 Japanese-speaking healthy older adults between 45 and 60 years of age (male/female = 35/36). Right-handed participants, as determined by the Japanese version of the Flinders Handedness survey (FLANDERS), were included [49]. Participants with minimal body movement during the rCBF measurement were preferentially included. The exclusion criteria were as follows: visual or hearing impediments, suspected dementia, anamnesis of cranial nerve disease, diagnosis of depression or depressive symptoms, menopausal symptoms or hormone treatment, irregular lifestyle (e.g., shift work), high consumption of alcohol (> 20 g/day), use of cigarettes, experience of the neuropsychological tests within 1 year, current treatment for cognitive functions, regular consumption of drugs or health functional supplements affecting cognitive functions including docosahexaenoic acids and gingko (> once a week), regular consumption of protein supplements (> once a week), anamnesis of severe disease requiring regular treatment, allergy or sensitivity to milk, and pregnancy or breastfeeding. The inclusion and exclusion criteria were checked during the screening steps using a questionnaire, Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (excluded if score ≤25), interviews by the principal investigator, and the data of rCBF measurement. Previous studies evaluating other functional ingredients, such as gingko, docosahexaenoic acids, and resveratrol, required 10–30 participants to detect statistically significant differences in rCBF (α = 0.05) [42, 50, 51]. To ensure sufficient statistical power after stratification, at least 25 participants were required per group.

Experimental supplements

The test tablets containing 1.8 mg of β-lactolin in the whey enzymatic digestion were prepared by Kirin Holdings Co., Ltd (Tokyo, Japan) according to the previous study [6]. The amount of β-lactolin, one of the Trp-Tyr-related β-lactopeptides, was measured by the LC/MS/MS method using synthetic GTWY peptide (Peptide Institute, inc., Osaka, Japan) as a standard. The tablets were ingested every day for 6 weeks. As a previous work showed that supplementations with β-lactolin for 6 weeks improved the cognitive function, in we supplemented β-lactolin for this specific period. In placebo tablets, whey enzymatic digestions were substituted with the same amount of maltodextrin. The test and placebo tablets were indistinguishable by size, shape, and taste. The amount and periods of β-lactolin supplementation and tablet composition were equivalent to the previous trials because the current study aimed to investigate the mechanism of the previous findings [6, 14].

Procedures

The trial was performed according to a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind, parallel-group comparative design. Questionnaires for the inclusion/exclusion criteria and lifestyle data, measurements of blood pressure, height, body weight, MMSE, rCBF, and medical interviews for safety assessments by a principal investigator were performed during the screening step. Selected participants were randomly allocated to the β-lactolin or placebo group with a computer program that ensured similar age, sex, MMSE, and oxy-Hb values between the study groups. The study group allocator was not involved in the assessment of eligibility, data collection, or analysis. The participants, research staff, and outcome assessors were blinded to group allocations until data analyses had been completed. During the intervention period, the participants were instructed to maintain regular lifestyles, avoid taking any drugs and new health functional supplements affecting cognitive function or CBF, and avoid taking protein supplements. Compliance was monitored using participant diaries. On the day of the rCBF measurement, the participants were instructed to completely avoid consuming caffeine and ingesting any foods and beverages, except for water, 2 h prior to the start of the tests. Data were collected at the Breast-health clinic (Tokyo, Japan) between January and March 2019. This study was conducted by the contract research organization, KT Medical Co., Ltd.

Measurements of cerebral blood flow (Primary outcome)

The rCBF was measured using a 34CH NIRS system (WOT-HS34M, NeU Corporation, Tokyo, Japan), which used continuous wave laser diodes with two wavelengths (730 and 850 nm) as light sources and was able to detect concentration changes in oxy-Hb, deoxygenated hemoglobin (deoxy-Hb), and their sum (total-Hb). The transmitted light was detected every 100 ms with avalanche photodiodes located at 30 mm from the sources, forming 34 measurement channels. The probes of the NIRS were placed on the participants' frontal region, with the lowest probes being positioned along the Fp1-Fp2 line, as defined by the international 10–20 system used in electroencephalography. This probe arrangement can measure the Hb changes in the approximate surface regions bilaterally in the DLPFC (Brodmann’s areas 9 and 46), which was cover at CH11, CH13 and CH14 in the right side and CH20, CH22 and CH23 in the left side. The participants were fitted with the NIRS headset, of which the probe position was in accordance with the previous report, and the rules of tasks were explained to them as described below [52]. Moreover, the participants completed a 5-min rest period and, then, completed the task session. They performed eight, eight, one, and one trials of verbal working memory, spatial working memory, serial 7s, and RVIP tasks, respectively, as described in the following section, for a total of approximately 20 min (Supplementary Figure 2). Each session was separated by a short break (approximately 30 s). At weeks 0 and 6 of the intervention, the procedures measuring rCBF were performed in the same way. Τhe participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a quiet room and instructed to maintain this position during the tasks.

Data analysis of cerebral blood flow

Analyses were performed using a plug-in-based analysis software, the platform for optical topography analysis tools (developed by Hitachi ARL.; run on MATLAB, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Oxy-Hb was the most sensitive variable to concentration changes in the regional rCBF, and provided the strongest correlation with the BOLD signal among the three NIRS parameters [53, 54]. Thus, we analyzed the changes in the oxy-Hb levels, as the best indicator of brain activity. In the present study, the changes of oxy-Hb were considered as those of the CBF. First, we calculated the relative values of concentration changes in oxy-Hb for each channel based on the modified Beer-Lambert law, using light signals transmitted at the two wavelengths. These signals were expressed as the product of the changes in Hb concentration (mM) and optical path length (mm) in the activation region (effective optical path length).

The NIRS system measured changes in the concentration of oxy-Hb and deoxy-Hb from the starting baseline. The baseline was obtained from the first (1.0) and the last (10.0) s of each block and was described in detail as follows. A Butterworth band-pass filter (0.02–0.80 Hz) was, then, applied to the oxy-Hb signals to remove low-frequency drift/oscillation and high-frequency system noise. In the verbal and spatial working memory tasks, the time-continuous data of Hb-signals were divided into 24.5-s task blocks, which consisted of a 1.0-s pre-task period (starting at 1.0 s before task period), an 8.5-s task period, and a 15.0-s post-task period (starting after task period). Similarly, in the serial 7s task, a set of 196.0 s task blocks, which consisted of a 1.0-s pre-task, a 180.0 s-task, and a 15.0-s post-task period. In the RVIP task, a set of 216.0-s task blocks, which consisted of 1.0-s pre-task, 200.0-s task, and 15.0-s post-task periods. Median filter methods were used to remove minor motion artifacts (window size, 2.0 s), and a baseline correction was performed using linear fitting based on two baseline periods: the first (1.0) s and the last (10.0) s of each block. For the verbal and spatial working memory tasks, individual time-course data for the oxy-Hb concentrations were averaged over the eight trials of each task. The data from each channel were converted into z-scores to reduce the impact of participant and channel differences and to compare appropriately [55]. The z-score was calculated using the mean and standard deviation of changes in the oxy-Hb concentrations. Consequently, the mean and the standard deviation were adjusted to a z-score of 0 and 1 for every channel, respectively. In this study, the ROI regarding the NIRS data were arranged into two regions: the right (CH 11, 13, and 14) and the left DLPFC area (CH 20, 22, and 23) [52, 56, 57]. Six channels were analyzed during the working memory tasks in the present study.

Cognitive tasks

During measurements of rCBF, the participants performed four cognitive tasks: the verbal working memory, the spatial working memory [52, 58], the serial subtraction, and the RVIP [59, 60]. Before each task, the participants performed short trials to become familiarized with the procedures.

The verbal and the spatial working memory tasks were presented through a stimulus presentation system (SP-POST01; NeU Corporation, Tokyo, Japan), which provided an identical delayed-response paradigm (Supplementary Figure 3). In both tasks, each trial started with a 1.5 s presentation of the target stimuli (S1), which was followed by a 7.0 s delay. A probe stimulus (S2) was, then, presented for 2.0 s or until the participants responded. The reset intervals between S2 and the next S1 were from 16 to 24 s. The mark of a cross was presented on the center of the screen during the reset intervals. In addition, a visual cue (changing the color of the fixation cross) was presented for 0.5 s prior to target stimulation (S1) and auditory cues (1000 and 800 Hz pure tones of 100 ms duration) were presented prior to S1 and S2.

In the verbal working memory, four Japanese characters in Hiragana were presented as S1 in Supplementary Figure 2, and a Japanese character in Katakana was presented as S2 in Supplementary Figure 2. The participants were instructed to judge whether the character presented as S2 corresponded to any of the characters in S1 and, then, to press the appropriate button as quickly as possible. As the characters in S1 and S2 were presented in different Japanese morphograms (i.e., Hiragana/Katakana), the participants were prompted to make their judgments based on the phonetic information of the characters, not by their form. The task was scored for the shortest reaction and the average reaction time.

In the spatial working memory task, S1 was the location of four red squares out of eight locations, and S2 was the location of a red square (Supplementary Figure 2). The participants decided whether the red location presented in S2 was included in the S1. The shortest and average reaction times were recorded.

The serial subtraction and the RVIP task were presented on a touchpad using the Computerized Mental Performance Assessment System software (Northumbria University, Newcastle, UK). In the serial 7s, the participants had to keep subtracting 7 from a random starting number between 800 and 999 as quickly as possible. The starting number was cleared by the entry of the first response, and the three-digits thereafter were displayed as asterisks. The task’s duration was 3 min and was scored for the number of correct and total responses. In RVIP, the participants monitored a continuous series of single digits to respond to three consecutive odds or even digits as quickly as possible, which were projected on the screen at a rate of 100/min in a pseudo-random order for 3 min. The task was scored for the correct answer rate and the average reaction time.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses of the rCBF were performed for six channels of the ROI using the R statistical software (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). Statistical significance of the rCBF and task performance were evaluated by a mixed-design two-way ANOVA with a between-participant factor of treatment (β-lactolin and placebo) and a within-subject factor of period (pre and post). We used the false discovery rate (FDR) method to correct for multiple comparisons [61] with a threshold of FDR (q) < 0.05. Statistical analyses of other data were performed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and differences between the groups were identified using unpaired t-tests. The comparisons of the test period (pre and post) were examined with the paired t-test. To evaluate the correlations between the oxy-Hb signal changes and task performance, Spearman’s rank correlation was performed according to the previous report [62]. Differences were considered statistically significant when p < 0.05 or q < 0.05.

Author Contributions

Y.A. and K.K. conducted the study.; M.H. conducted the study as principal investigator; K.K. and Y.A. wrote the manuscript.; R.K. and Y.A. designed and supervised the experiments. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate Mr. Kuniaki Obara for his assistance to manage this clinical project. We also appreciate NeU Corporation originated from HITACHI High-Tech Corporation for the technical support and consult to set the experimental parameters and measure the rCBF using multichannel NIRS.

Conflicts of Interest

K.K. and Y.A. are employees of Kirin Holdings Company, Ltd., the study sponsor. All authors have declared that they have no other disclosures. This study was funded by the Kirin Holdings Company, Ltd. The funder designed this study and supplied the test tablets and placebo. The funder, however, had no role in data collection or analysis. The manuscript was prepared by members of the funder and the funder decided to publish the results.

Funding

This study was funded by the Kirin Holdings Company, Limited.

References

  • 1. Crichton GE, Bryan J, Murphy KJ, Buckley J. Review of dairy consumption and cognitive performance in adults: findings and methodological issues. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010; 30:352–61. https://doi.org/10.1159/000320987 [PubMed]
  • 2. Ozawa M, Ninomiya T, Ohara T, Doi Y, Uchida K, Shirota T, Yonemoto K, Kitazono T, Kiyohara Y. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia in an elderly Japanese population: the hisayama study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013; 97:1076–82. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.112.045575 [PubMed]
  • 3. Ozawa M, Ohara T, Ninomiya T, Hata J, Yoshida D, Mukai N, Nagata M, Uchida K, Shirota T, Kitazono T, Kiyohara Y. Milk and dairy consumption and risk of dementia in an elderly Japanese population: the hisayama study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014; 62:1224–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.12887 [PubMed]
  • 4. Ano Y, Nakayama H. Preventive effects of dairy products on dementia and the underlying mechanisms. Int J Mol Sci. 2018; 19:1927. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms19071927 [PubMed]
  • 5. Ano Y, Ayabe T, Kutsukake T, Ohya R, Takaichi Y, Uchida S, Yamada K, Uchida K, Takashima A, Nakayama H. Novel lactopeptides in fermented dairy products improve memory function and cognitive decline. Neurobiol Aging. 2018; 72:23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.07.016 [PubMed]
  • 6. Kita M, Kobayashi K, Obara K, Koikeda T, Umeda S, Ano Y. Supplementation with whey peptide rich in β-lactolin improves cognitive performance in healthy older adults: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Front Neurosci. 2019; 13:399. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00399 [PubMed]
  • 7. Ano Y, Ozawa M, Kutsukake T, Sugiyama S, Uchida K, Yoshida A, Nakayama H. Preventive effects of a fermented dairy product against Alzheimer’s disease and identification of a novel oleamide with enhanced microglial phagocytosis and anti-inflammatory activity. PLoS One. 2015; 10:e0118512. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118512 [PubMed]
  • 8. Ano Y, Yoshino Y, Kutsukake T, Ohya R, Fukuda T, Uchida K, Takashima A, Nakayama H. Tryptophan-related dipeptides in fermented dairy products suppress microglial activation and prevent cognitive decline. Aging (Albany NY). 2019; 11:2949–2967. https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.101909 [PubMed]
  • 9. Ano Y, Nakayama H. Novel lacto-peptides improve cognitive decline. Aging (Albany NY). 2019; 11:1615–16. https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.101893 [PubMed]
  • 10. Ano Y, Ohya R, Kita M, Taniguchi Y, Kondo K. Theaflavins improve memory impairment and depression-like behavior by regulating microglial activation. Molecules. 2019; 24:467. https://doi.org/10.3390/molecules24030467 [PubMed]
  • 11. Ayabe T, Ohya R, Ano Y. Β-lactolin, a whey-derived glycine-threonine-tryptophan-tyrosine lactotetrapeptide, improves prefrontal cortex-associated reversal learning in mice. Biosci Biotechnol Biochem. 2020; 84:1039–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/09168451.2020.1714424 [PubMed]
  • 12. Ayabe T, Ano Y, Ohya R, Kitaoka S, Furuyashiki T. The lacto-tetrapeptide gly-thr-trp-tyr, β-lactolin, improves spatial memory functions via dopamine release and D1 receptor activation in the hippocampus. Nutrients. 2019; 11:2469. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102469 [PubMed]
  • 13. Ano Y, Ohya R, Takaichi Y, Washinuma T, Uchida K, Takashima A, Nakayama H. Β-lactolin, a whey-derived lacto-tetrapeptide, prevents Alzheimer’s disease pathologies and cognitive decline. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020; 73:1331–42. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-190997 [PubMed]
  • 14. Kita M, Obara K, Kondo S, Umeda S, Ano Y. Effect of supplementation of a whey peptide rich in tryptophan-tyrosine-related peptides on cognitive performance in healthy adults: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Nutrients. 2018; 10:899. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10070899 [PubMed]
  • 15. Yuan P, Raz N. Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014; 42:180–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005 [PubMed]
  • 16. MacLeod CM, MacDonald PA. Interdimensional interference in the stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000; 4:383–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01530-8 [PubMed]
  • 17. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000; 41:49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [PubMed]
  • 18. Boushel R, Langberg H, Olesen J, Gonzales-Alonzo J, Bülow J, Kjaer M. Monitoring tissue oxygen availability with near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) in health and disease. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2001; 11:213–22. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0838.2001.110404.x [PubMed]
  • 19. Tseng YL, Lu CF, Wu SM, Shimada S, Huang T, Lu GY. A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study of state anxiety and auditory working memory load. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 12:313. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00313 [PubMed]
  • 20. Jahani S, Fantana AL, Harper D, Ellison JM, Boas DA, Forester BP, Yücel MA. fNIRS can robustly measure brain activity during memory encoding and retrieval in healthy subjects. Sci Rep. 2017; 7:9533. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09868-w [PubMed]
  • 21. Baker JM, Bruno JL, Gundran A, Hosseini SM, Reiss AL. fNIRS measurement of cortical activation and functional connectivity during a visuospatial working memory task. PLoS One. 2018; 13:e0201486. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201486 [PubMed]
  • 22. Elliott R. Executive functions and their disorders. Br Med Bull. 2003; 65:49–59. https://doi.org/10.1093/bmb/65.1.49 [PubMed]
  • 23. Monsell S. Task switching. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003; 7:134–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00028-7 [PubMed]
  • 24. Chan RC, Shum D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EY. Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008; 23:201–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010 [PubMed]
  • 25. Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994; 6:348–57. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.6.4.348 [PubMed]
  • 26. Brosnan MB, Wiegand I. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a dynamic cortical area to enhance top-down attentional control. J Neurosci. 2017; 37:3445–46. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0136-17.2017 [PubMed]
  • 27. Kaller CP, Heinze K, Frenkel A, Läppchen CH, Unterrainer JM, Weiller C, Lange R, Rahm B. Differential impact of continuous theta-burst stimulation over left and right DLPFC on planning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013; 34:36–51. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21423 [PubMed]
  • 28. Cools R, Froböse M, Aarts E, Hofmans L. Dopamine and the motivation of cognitive control. Handb Clin Neurol. 2019; 163:123–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00007-0 [PubMed]
  • 29. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Illowsky BP. Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Iii. A new cohort and evidence for a monoaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988; 45:609–15. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800310013001 [PubMed]
  • 30. Koenigs M, Grafman J. The functional neuroanatomy of depression: distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2009; 201:239–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.004 [PubMed]
  • 31. Arnsten AF, Raskind MA, Taylor FB, Connor DF. The effects of stress exposure on prefrontal cortex: translating basic research into successful treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Stress. 2015; 1:89–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2014.10.002 [PubMed]
  • 32. Kumar S, Zomorrodi R, Ghazala Z, Goodman MS, Blumberger DM, Cheam A, Fischer C, Daskalakis ZJ, Mulsant BH, Pollock BG, Rajji TK. Extent of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex plasticity and its association with working memory in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017; 74:1266–74. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3292 [PubMed]
  • 33. Nagahama Y, Fukuyama H, Yamauchi H, Katsumi Y, Magata Y, Shibasaki H, Kimura J. Age-related changes in cerebral blood flow activation during a card sorting test. Exp Brain Res. 1997; 114:571–77. https://doi.org/10.1007/pl00005665 [PubMed]
  • 34. Kazumata K, Tokairin K, Sugiyama T, Ito M, Uchino H, Osanai T, Kawabori M, Nakayama N, Houkin K. Association of cognitive function with cerebral blood flow in children with moyamoya disease. J Neurosurg Pediatr. 2019. [Epub ahead of print]. https://doi.org/10.3171/2019.7.PEDS19312 [PubMed]
  • 35. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Jonides J, Smith EE, Hartley A, Miller A, Marshuetz C, Koeppe RA. Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000; 12:174–87. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561814 [PubMed]
  • 36. Coull JT, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM. A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia. 1996; 34:1085–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(96)00029-2 [PubMed]
  • 37. Boggio PS, Ferrucci R, Mameli F, Martins D, Martins O, Vergari M, Tadini L, Scarpini E, Fregni F, Priori A. Prolonged visual memory enhancement after direct current stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Stimul. 2012; 5:223–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.006 [PubMed]
  • 38. Boggio PS, Khoury LP, Martins DC, Martins OE, de Macedo EC, Fregni F. Temporal cortex direct current stimulation enhances performance on a visual recognition memory task in Alzheimer disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009; 80:444–47. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2007.141853 [PubMed]
  • 39. Chang CH, Lane HY, Lin CH. Brain stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Front Psychiatry. 2018; 9:201. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00201 [PubMed]
  • 40. Du L, Liu H, Du W, Chao F, Zhang L, Wang K, Huang C, Gao Y, Tang Y. Stimulated left DLPFC-nucleus accumbens functional connectivity predicts the anti-depression and anti-anxiety effects of rTMS for depression. Transl Psychiatry. 2018; 7:3. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-017-0005-6 [PubMed]
  • 41. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Nouchi R, Hashizume H, Sekiguchi A, Kotozaki Y, Nakagawa S, Miyauchi CM, Sassa Y, Kawashima R. Effects of working memory training on functional connectivity and cerebral blood flow during rest. Cortex. 2013; 49:2106–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.007 [PubMed]
  • 42. Santos RF, Galduróz JC, Barbieri A, Castiglioni ML, Ytaya LY, Bueno OF. Cognitive performance, SPECT, and blood viscosity in elderly non-demented people using ginkgo biloba. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2003; 36:127–33. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2003-41197 [PubMed]
  • 43. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Goldstein RZ. Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: insight from imaging studies. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002; 78:610–24. https://doi.org/10.1006/nlme.2002.4099 [PubMed]
  • 44. Ott T, Nieder A. Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019; 23:213–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.006 [PubMed]
  • 45. Williams GV, Castner SA. Under the curve: critical issues for elucidating D1 receptor function in working memory. Neuroscience. 2006; 139:263–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.09.028 [PubMed]
  • 46. Bäckman L, Karlsson S, Fischer H, Karlsson P, Brehmer Y, Rieckmann A, MacDonald SW, Farde L, Nyberg L. Dopamine D(1) receptors and age differences in brain activation during working memory. Neurobiol Aging. 2011; 32:1849–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.10.018 [PubMed]
  • 47. Cools R, Stefanova E, Barker RA, Robbins TW, Owen AM. Dopaminergic modulation of high-level cognition in Parkinson’s disease: the role of the prefrontal cortex revealed by PET. Brain. 2002; 125:584–94. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf052 [PubMed]
  • 48. Ano Y, Ayabe T, Ohya R, Kondo K, Kitaoka S, Furuyashiki T. Tryptophan-tyrosine dipeptide, the core sequence of β-lactolin, improves memory by modulating the dopamine system. Nutrients. 2019; 11:348. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11020348 [PubMed]
  • 49. Okubo M, Suzuki H, Nicholls ME. [A Japanese version of the FLANDERS handedness questionnaire]. Shinrigaku Kenkyu. 2014; 85:474–81. https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.85.13235 [PubMed]
  • 50. Jackson PA, Reay JL, Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. DHA-rich oil modulates the cerebral haemodynamic response to cognitive tasks in healthy young adults: a near IR spectroscopy pilot study. Br J Nutr. 2012; 107:1093–98. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114511004041 [PubMed]
  • 51. Wightman EL, Haskell-Ramsay CF, Reay JL, Williamson G, Dew T, Zhang W, Kennedy DO. The effects of chronic trans-resveratrol supplementation on aspects of cognitive function, mood, sleep, health and cerebral blood flow in healthy, young humans. Br J Nutr. 2015; 114:1427–37. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114515003037 [PubMed]
  • 52. Aoki R, Sato H, Katura T, Utsugi K, Koizumi H, Matsuda R, Maki A. Relationship of negative mood with prefrontal cortex activity during working memory tasks: an optical topography study. Neurosci Res. 2011; 70:189–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.02.011 [PubMed]
  • 53. Hoshi Y, Kobayashi N, Tamura M. Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2001; 90:1657–62. https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.1657 [PubMed]
  • 54. Strangman G, Culver JP, Thompson JH, Boas DA. A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation. Neuroimage. 2002; 17:719–31. [PubMed]
  • 55. Matsuda G, Hiraki K. Sustained decrease in oxygenated hemoglobin during video games in the dorsal prefrontal cortex: a NIRS study of children. Neuroimage. 2006; 29:706–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.019 [PubMed]
  • 56. Atsumori H, Kiguchi M, Katura T, Funane T, Obata A, Sato H, Manaka T, Iwamoto M, Maki A, Koizumi H, Kubota K. Noninvasive imaging of prefrontal activation during attention-demanding tasks performed while walking using a wearable optical topography system. J Biomed Opt. 2010; 15:046002. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.3462996 [PubMed]
  • 57. Pinti P, Merla A, Aichelburg C, Lind F, Power S, Swingler E, Hamilton A, Gilbert S, Burgess PW, Tachtsidis I. A novel GLM-based method for the automatic IDentification of functional events (AIDE) in fNIRS data recorded in naturalistic environments. Neuroimage. 2017; 155:291–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.001 [PubMed]
  • 58. Sato H, Yahata N, Funane T, Takizawa R, Katura T, Atsumori H, Nishimura Y, Kinoshita A, Kiguchi M, Koizumi H, Fukuda M, Kasai K. A NIRS-fMRI investigation of prefrontal cortex activity during a working memory task. Neuroimage. 2013; 83:158–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.043 [PubMed]
  • 59. Kennedy DO, Wightman EL, Reay JL, Lietz G, Okello EJ, Wilde A, Haskell CF. Effects of resveratrol on cerebral blood flow variables and cognitive performance in humans: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010; 91:1590–97. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2009.28641 [PubMed]
  • 60. Jackson PA, Reay JL, Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. Docosahexaenoic acid-rich fish oil modulates the cerebral hemodynamic response to cognitive tasks in healthy young adults. Biol Psychol. 2012; 89:183–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.006 [PubMed]
  • 61. Singh AK, Dan I. Exploring the false discovery rate in multichannel NIRS. Neuroimage. 2006; 33:542–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.047 [PubMed]
  • 62. Schroeter ML, Zysset S, Wahl M, von Cramon DY. Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development—an event-related fNIRS study. Neuroimage. 2004; 23:1317–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.001 [PubMed]